Cognitive scientist Lera Boroditsky with Pormpuraaw children |
Linguistic
determinism, the stronger form of linguistic relativity, claims that the
structure of language determines how people think, and more extremely, the
language we speak constrains us to certain thoughts. Whorf, as a main proponent
of this principle, tested the hypothesis with his pioneering work comparing
Hopi and English speakers in the 1930s. With this experiment, Whorf argued that
the Hopi Native American tribe speaker conceptualized time differently from the
Standard Average European speaker due to grammatical differences between the
languages (“Hopi Time Controversy”). After analyzing his observations, Whorf
concluded that in Hopi perspective, “time disappears and space is altered, so
that no longer the homogeneous and instantaneous timeless space of our supposed
intuition or of classical Newtonian mechanics” (28). As opposed to the way
English speakers divide time into past, present, and future tenses, the Hopi
lack this vocabulary, thus making it impossible for them to think of time. While
the Whorfian view was largely criticized in the following years by linguists
and anthropologists, it lived on as an urban myth that “the Hopi have no
concept of time”.
In the dystopian novel 1984, author George Orwell incorporates the theme of linguistic determinism
as a tool for totalitarian rule (Blackmore 7). With the intent of eradicating
concepts of political or intellectual freedom, the government in 1984 replaces “Oldspeak,” the English
that exists in our world, with “Newspeak,” a modified version of English with a
much smaller vocabulary (Deol). As “Newspeak
was designed not to extend but to diminish the range of thought,” the Party’s
ideology of limiting a person’s ability to think serves both as a fictional
interpretation and a warning to the implications of linguistic determinism
(Orwell 1). While the theory of linguistic determinism supports the idea that a
totalitarian government can hold power over our ability to think of certain
concepts, this became unlikely as another theory, claiming universal
language-related processes, gained popularity and debunked the Sapir-Whorf
hypothesis.
The ignorance given to linguistic relativity in the second half of the
twentieth century is largely credited to Noam Chomsky’s idea of universal
grammatical systems. Chomsky’s theory imposed that in order to learn any
language, children forming grammatical sentences at an early age have to have
an inborn set of underlying rules. In developing his theory, Chomsky combined
the computational approach to language with human biology, promising new
discoveries in the foundations of all the world’s human languages. Chomsky’s
idea that children held an innate “Language Acquisition Device” gained an
immediate appeal to scientists (Malone 2). However, this concept of having “brains
hardwired with a mental template for learning grammar” had its flaws, as
certain grammatical elements or the lack thereof pertaining to several
non-European languages challenged the idea of universal grammar and was
recently abandoned by cognitive scientists and linguists due to new research in
various languages as well as the way children learn to speak the languages of
their societies (Ibbotson and Tomasello).
After a brief hiatus in
the acceptance of linguistic relativity in the sixties and seventies, new
research has advocated the weaker form of the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis. As
Boroditsky claims, empirical evidence showing how languages shape thinking is
finally being recognized, overturning the idea about universality (63).
Language shapes the most essential dimensions of human experience—space, time,
causality and relationships to others. The work of psycholinguists Stephen C.
Levinson and John B. Haviland demonstrates that people who speak languages that
involve the use of directions are good at keeping track of their location, even
in unfamiliar regions. In addition, research conducted by Boroditsky herself
and Alice Gaby where they gave Kuuk Thaayorre speakers picture cards to
organize in correct temporal order revealed that those who think differently
about space may probably think differently about time, as the Kuuk Thaayorre
arranged the cards from east to west, instead of from left to right like
English speakers or from right to left like Hebrew speakers, implying that
writing direction in a language also influences how we organize time (64).
In addition, Danziger and
Ward advocate the “numerous findings showing an influence of language structure
on cognition” (799). In their experiment, Danziger and Ward tested whether Arab
Israelis’ attitudes towards Arabs and Jews differed depending on if the
associations were instigated in Hebrew or Arabic (there is a tension in the
multilingual and multicultural community of Israel). The results showed that
language use “can selectively influence the accessibility of socially relevant
associations,” as well as proving the statements of language and culture being
intricately linked and bilingual people thinking about their social
circumstance in a different way depending on their current language context
(800). By using bilingual subjects, this demonstrates a valid direct
correlation in culture and language to cognition. Thus, while linguistic
determinism and universal grammar structures are eliminated, the weaker form of
linguistic relativity is especially prevalent.
While the reality of a
totalitarian government coming to power one day and controlling our thoughts is
unlikely, the weaker form of linguistic relativity can be seen in our everyday
lives through our unconscious use of language. Similarly, the idea of innate
universal language structure may excite some that are looking for a scientific
explanation to humans’ language evolution over time; however, the notion of
language, thought, and culture being tied together opens new doors in the
disciplines of psychology and linguistics. Hence, if our perception is
influenced by the language we speak, would a person that learned to speak
multiple languages have a wider understanding of the world? The implementation of
the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis in language acquisition practices can help transform
the attitudes of our world, collectively. When language is looked upon from this perspective, it can be seen as reflection of the values and thoughts that people in a culture share based on the words they chose to create, and that is beautiful.
Works
Cited
Blackmore, Ashley. “Revitalising Linguistic Relativity.”
2012, www.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:531920/FULLTEXT01.pdf.
Boroditsky, Lera, and Alice Gaby. “Remembrances of Times
East: Absolute Spatial Representations of Time in an Australian Aboriginal
Community.” Psychological Science, vol. 21, no. 11, 2010, pp. 1635–1639. JSTOR,
JSTOR, www.jstor.org/stable/41062425.
Boroditsky, Lera. “How Language Shapes Thought.” Scientific American, vol. 304, no. 2,
2011, pp. 62–65. JSTOR, JSTOR, www.jstor.org/stable/26002395.
Danziger, Shai, and Robert Ward. “Language Changes Implicit
Associations Between Ethnic Groups and Evaluation in Bilinguals.” Psychological
Science, vol. 21, no. 6, 2010, pp. 799–800. JSTOR, JSTOR,
www.jstor.org/stable/41062292.
Deol, Justin. “Can You Think Complex Thoughts Without
Language? | 1984 - George Orwell.” Freedom
in Thought, March 2018, www.freedominthought.com/archive/can-you-think-complex-thoughts-without-language-1984-george-orwell.
“Hopi Time Controversy Explained.” Explained.Today, everything.explained.today/Hopi_time_controversy/.
Ibbotson, Paul, and Michael Tomasello. “Evidence Rebuts
Chomsky’s Theory of Language Learning.” Scientific
American, November 2016,
www.scientificamerican.com/article/evidence-rebuts-chomsky-s-theory-of-language-learning/.
Koerner, E. F. Konrad. “The Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis: A
Preliminary History and a Bibliographical Essay.” Journal of Linguistic
Anthropology, vol. 2, no. 2, 1992, pp. 173–198. JSTOR, JSTOR, www.jstor.org/stable/43102168.
Malone, Dennis L. “Theories and Research of Second Language
Acquisition.” SIL International,
2012,
www.sil.org/sites/default/files/files/theories_and_research_of_second_language_acquisition.pdf.
Orwell, George. 1984.
Penguin Books, 2008.
WHORF, BENJAMIN LEE. “AN AMERICAN INDIAN MODEL OF THE
UNIVERSE.” ETC: A Review of General
Semantics, vol. 8, no. 1, 1950, pp. 27–33. JSTOR, JSTOR, www.jstor.org/stable/42581334.
No comments:
Post a Comment